Quickly Grasping Naturalistic Morality

In my opinion, to better grasp, a naturalistic morality one should see the perspective of moral reasoning in the self-awareness analyzing process in striving to reach moral conclusions. As in how there is a self-regulatory effect on the self-evaluative moral emotions, such as shame and guilt. Broadly understood or explained, self-regulation on self-evaluative moral emotions can be distinguished into two types of general motivations: approach/activation and avoidance/inhibition. One should conceptually understand the socialization dimensions (restrictiveness versus nurturance), associated emotions (anxiety versus empathy), and forms of morality (proscriptive versus prescriptive) that serve as precursors to each self-evaluative moral emotion and thus the moral conclusion and following choice actions. ref, ref, ref, ref

Moral emotions

“Moral emotions are a variety of social emotion that are involved in forming and communicating moral judgments and decisions, and in motivating behavioral responses to one’s own and others’ moral behavior. Moral reasoning has been the focus of most study of morality dating all the way back to Plato and Aristotle. The emotive side of morality has been looked upon with disdain, as subservient to the higher, rational, moral reasoning, with scholars like Piaget and Kohlberg touting moral reasoning as the key forefront of morality. However, in the last 30–40 years, there has been a rise in a new front of research: moral emotions as the basis for moral behavior. This development began with a focus on empathy and guilt, but has since moved on to encompass new emotional scholarship on emotions such as angershamedisgustawe, and elevation. With the new research, theorists have begun to question whether moral emotions might hold a larger role in determining morality, one that might even surpass that of moral reasoning.” ref

“There have generally been two approaches taken by philosophers to define moral emotion. The first “is to specify the formal conditions that make a moral statement (e.g., that is prescriptive, that it is universal, such as expedience)”. This first approach is more tied to language and the definitions given to moral emotions. The second approach “is to specify the material conditions of a moral issue, for example, that moral rules and judgments ‘must bear on the interest or welfare either of society as a whole or at least of persons other than the judge or agent'”. This definition seems to be more action-based. It focuses on the outcome of a moral emotion. The second definition is more preferred because it is not tied to language and therefore can be applied to prelinguistic children and animals. Moral emotions are “emotions that are linked to the interests or welfare either of society as a whole or at least of persons other than the judge or agent.” ref

“There is a debate whether there is a set of basic emotions or if there are “scripts or set of components that can be mixed and matched, allowing for a very large number of possible emotions”. Even those arguing for a basic set acknowledge that there are variants of each emotion (psychologist Paul Ekman calls these variants “families”). According to Jonathan Haidt: “The principal moral emotions can be divided into two large and two small joint families. The large families are the “other-condemning” family, in which the three brothers are contemptanger, and disgust (and their many children, such as indignation and loathing), and the “self-conscious” family (shameembarrassment, and guilt)…[T]he two smaller families the “other-suffering” family (compassion) and the “other-praising” family (gratitude and elevation).” ref

“Haidt would suggest that the higher the emotionality of a moral agent, the more likely the agent is to act morally. He uses the term “disinterested elicitor” to describe an event or situation that provokes emotions in us even when these emotions do not have anything to do with our own personal welfare. It is these elicitors that cause people to participate in what he calls “prosocial action tendencies” (actions that benefit society). Haidt explains moral emotions as “emotion families”, in which each family contains emotions that may be similar although not exactly the same. These moral emotions are provoked by eliciting events that often lead to prosocial action tendencies. Each person’s likelihood of prosocial action is determined by his or her degree of emotionality.” ref

“Empathy also plays a large role in altruism. The empathy-altruism hypothesis states that feelings of empathy for another leads to an altruistic motivation to help that person. In contrast, there may also be an egoistic motivation to help someone in need. This is the Hullian tension-reduction model in which personal distress caused by another in need leads the person to help in order to alleviate their own discomfort. The Altruism Born of Suffering Literature states that individuals who have undergone difficult times and grown from this trauma identify with seeing others in need and respond altruistically by protecting or caring for others. In the context of climate change, it is recognised that for individuals to act altruistically towards their society and environment, they need to learn to increase their capacity to process their emotional experiences as well as increased reflective functioning.” ref

“Batson, Klein, Highberger, and Shaw conducted experiments where they manipulated people through the use of empathy-induced altruism to make decisions that required them to show partiality to one individual over another. The first experiment involved a participant from each group to choose someone to experience a positive or negative task. These groups included a non-communication, communication/low-empathy, and communication/high-empathy. They were asked to make their decisions based on these standards resulting in the communication/high-empathy group showing more partiality in the experiment than the other groups due to being successfully manipulated emotionally. Those individuals who they successfully manipulated reported that despite feeling compelled in the moment to show partiality, they still felt they had made the more “immoral” decision since they followed an empathy-based emotion rather than adhering to a justice perspective of morality.” ref

“Batson, Klein, Highberger, & Shaw conducted two experiments on empathy-induced altruism, proposing that this can lead to actions that violate the justice principle. The second experiment operated similarly to the first using low-empathy and high-empathy groups. Participants were faced with the decision to move an ostensibly ill child to an “immediate help” group versus leaving her on a waiting list after listening to her emotionally-driven interview describing her condition and the life it has left her to lead. Those who were in the high-empathy group were more likely than those in the low-empathy group to move the child higher up the list to receive treatment earlier. When these participants were asked what the more moral choice was, they agreed that the more moral choice would have been to not move this child ahead of the list at the expense of the other children. In this case, it is evident that when empathy-induced altruism is at odds with what is seen as moral, oftentimes empathy induced altruism has the ability to win out over morality.” ref

“Recently neuroscientist Jean Decety, drawing on empirical research in evolutionary theorydevelopmental psychologysocial neuroscience, and psychopathy, argued that empathy and morality are neither systematically opposed to one another, nor inevitably complementary. Emmons (2009) defines gratitude as a natural emotional reaction and a universal tendency to respond positively to another’s benevolence. Gratitude is motivating and leads to what Emmons’ describes as “upstream reciprocity”. This is the passing on of benefits to third parties instead of returning benefits to one’s benefactors (Emmons, 2009). In the context of social networking behavior, research from Brady, Wills, Jost, Tucker, and Van Bavel (2017) shows that the expression of moral emotion amplifies the extent to which moral and political ideals are disseminated in social media platforms. Analyzing a large sample of Twitter communications on polarizing issues, such as gun control, same-sex marriage, and climate change, results indicated that the presence of moral-emotional language in messages increased their transmission by approximately 20% per word, compared to purely-moral and purely-emotional language.” ref

Pin It on Pinterest

Share This